Wiki source code of Studies: Crime and Substance Abuse
Show last authors
author | version | line-number | content |
---|---|---|---|
1 | = Crime and Substance Abuse = | ||
2 | |||
3 | {{expandable summary="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"}} | ||
4 | **Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* | ||
5 | **Date of Publication:** *2002* | ||
6 | **Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti* | ||
7 | **Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"* | ||
8 | **DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424) | ||
9 | **Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts* | ||
10 | |||
11 | {{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} | ||
12 | 1. **General Observations:** | ||
13 | - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders. | ||
14 | - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**. | ||
15 | |||
16 | 2. **Subgroup Analysis:** | ||
17 | - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**. | ||
18 | - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities. | ||
19 | |||
20 | 3. **Other Significant Data Points:** | ||
21 | - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion. | ||
22 | - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**. | ||
23 | {{/expandable}} | ||
24 | |||
25 | {{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} | ||
26 | 1. **Primary Observations:** | ||
27 | - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success. | ||
28 | - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates. | ||
29 | |||
30 | 2. **Subgroup Trends:** | ||
31 | - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders. | ||
32 | - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**. | ||
33 | |||
34 | 3. **Specific Case Analysis:** | ||
35 | - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**. | ||
36 | - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**. | ||
37 | {{/expandable}} | ||
38 | |||
39 | {{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} | ||
40 | 1. **Strengths of the Study:** | ||
41 | - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**. | ||
42 | - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis. | ||
43 | |||
44 | 2. **Limitations of the Study:** | ||
45 | - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**. | ||
46 | - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**. | ||
47 | |||
48 | 3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** | ||
49 | - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**. | ||
50 | - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**. | ||
51 | {{/expandable}} | ||
52 | |||
53 | {{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} | ||
54 | - Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**. | ||
55 | - Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**. | ||
56 | - Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**. | ||
57 | {{/expandable}} | ||
58 | |||
59 | {{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} | ||
60 | 1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**. | ||
61 | 2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**. | ||
62 | 3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**. | ||
63 | {{/expandable}} | ||
64 | |||
65 | {{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} | ||
66 | [[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]] | ||
67 | {{/expandable}} | ||
68 | {{/expandable}} | ||
69 | |||
70 | {{expandable summary="Study: Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"}} | ||
71 | **Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* | ||
72 | **Date of Publication:** *2003* | ||
73 | **Author(s):** *Timothy P. Johnson, Phillip J. Bowman* | ||
74 | **Title:** *"Cross-Cultural Sources of Measurement Error in Substance Use Surveys"* | ||
75 | **DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120023394](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120023394) | ||
76 | **Subject Matter:** *Survey Methodology, Racial Disparities, Substance Use Research* | ||
77 | |||
78 | {{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} | ||
79 | 1. **General Observations:** | ||
80 | - Study examined **how racial and cultural factors influence self-reported substance use data**. | ||
81 | - Analyzed **36 empirical studies from 1977–2003** on survey reliability across racial/ethnic groups. | ||
82 | |||
83 | 2. **Subgroup Analysis:** | ||
84 | - Black and Latino respondents **were more likely to underreport drug use** compared to White respondents. | ||
85 | - **Cultural stigma and distrust in research institutions** affected self-report accuracy. | ||
86 | |||
87 | 3. **Other Significant Data Points:** | ||
88 | - **Surveys using biological validation (urinalysis, hair tests) revealed underreporting trends**. | ||
89 | - **Higher recantation rates** (denying past drug use) were observed among minority respondents. | ||
90 | {{/expandable}} | ||
91 | |||
92 | {{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} | ||
93 | 1. **Primary Observations:** | ||
94 | - Racial/ethnic disparities in **substance use reporting bias survey-based research**. | ||
95 | - **Social desirability and cultural norms impact data reliability**. | ||
96 | |||
97 | 2. **Subgroup Trends:** | ||
98 | - White respondents were **more likely to overreport** substance use. | ||
99 | - Black and Latino respondents **had higher recantation rates**, particularly in face-to-face interviews. | ||
100 | |||
101 | 3. **Specific Case Analysis:** | ||
102 | - Mode of survey administration **significantly influenced reporting accuracy**. | ||
103 | - **Self-administered surveys produced more reliable data than interviewer-administered surveys**. | ||
104 | {{/expandable}} | ||
105 | |||
106 | {{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} | ||
107 | 1. **Strengths of the Study:** | ||
108 | - **Comprehensive review of 36 studies** on measurement error in substance use reporting. | ||
109 | - Identifies **systemic biases affecting racial/ethnic survey reliability**. | ||
110 | |||
111 | 2. **Limitations of the Study:** | ||
112 | - Relies on **secondary data analysis**, limiting direct experimental control. | ||
113 | - Does not explore **how measurement error impacts policy decisions**. | ||
114 | |||
115 | 3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** | ||
116 | - Future research should **incorporate mixed-method approaches** (qualitative & quantitative). | ||
117 | - Investigate **how survey design can reduce racial reporting disparities**. | ||
118 | {{/expandable}} | ||
119 | |||
120 | {{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} | ||
121 | - Supports research on **racial disparities in self-reported health behaviors**. | ||
122 | - Highlights **survey methodology issues that impact substance use epidemiology**. | ||
123 | - Provides insights for **improving data accuracy in public health research**. | ||
124 | {{/expandable}} | ||
125 | |||
126 | {{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} | ||
127 | 1. Investigate **how survey design impacts racial disparities in self-reported health data**. | ||
128 | 2. Study **alternative data collection methods (biometric validation, passive data tracking)**. | ||
129 | 3. Explore **the role of social stigma in self-reported health behaviors**. | ||
130 | {{/expandable}} | ||
131 | |||
132 | {{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} | ||
133 | [[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120023394.pdf]] | ||
134 | {{/expandable}} | ||
135 | {{/expandable}} | ||
136 | |||
137 | {{expandable summary="Study: Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"}} | ||
138 | **Source:** *Substance Use & Misuse* | ||
139 | **Date of Publication:** *2002* | ||
140 | **Author(s):** *Clifford A. Butzin, Christine A. Saum, Frank R. Scarpitti* | ||
141 | **Title:** *"Factors Associated with Completion of a Drug Treatment Court Diversion Program"* | ||
142 | **DOI:** [10.1081/JA-120014424](https://doi.org/10.1081/JA-120014424) | ||
143 | **Subject Matter:** *Substance Use, Criminal Justice, Drug Courts* | ||
144 | |||
145 | {{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} | ||
146 | 1. **General Observations:** | ||
147 | - Study examined **drug treatment court success rates** among first-time offenders. | ||
148 | - Strongest predictors of **successful completion were employment status and race**. | ||
149 | |||
150 | 2. **Subgroup Analysis:** | ||
151 | - Individuals with **stable jobs were more likely to complete the program**. | ||
152 | - **Black participants had lower success rates**, suggesting potential systemic disparities. | ||
153 | |||
154 | 3. **Other Significant Data Points:** | ||
155 | - **Education level was positively correlated** with program completion. | ||
156 | - Frequency of **drug use before enrollment affected treatment outcomes**. | ||
157 | {{/expandable}} | ||
158 | |||
159 | {{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} | ||
160 | 1. **Primary Observations:** | ||
161 | - **Social stability factors** (employment, education) were key to treatment success. | ||
162 | - **Race and pre-existing substance use patterns** influenced completion rates. | ||
163 | |||
164 | 2. **Subgroup Trends:** | ||
165 | - White offenders had **higher completion rates** than Black offenders. | ||
166 | - Drug court success was **higher for those with lower initial drug use frequency**. | ||
167 | |||
168 | 3. **Specific Case Analysis:** | ||
169 | - **Individuals with strong social ties were more likely to finish the program**. | ||
170 | - Success rates were **significantly higher for participants with case management support**. | ||
171 | {{/expandable}} | ||
172 | |||
173 | {{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} | ||
174 | 1. **Strengths of the Study:** | ||
175 | - **First empirical study on drug court program success factors**. | ||
176 | - Uses **longitudinal data** for post-treatment analysis. | ||
177 | |||
178 | 2. **Limitations of the Study:** | ||
179 | - Lacks **qualitative data on personal motivation and treatment engagement**. | ||
180 | - Focuses on **short-term program success** without tracking **long-term relapse rates**. | ||
181 | |||
182 | 3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** | ||
183 | - Future research should examine **racial disparities in drug court outcomes**. | ||
184 | - Study **how community resources impact long-term recovery**. | ||
185 | {{/expandable}} | ||
186 | |||
187 | {{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} | ||
188 | - Provides insight into **what factors contribute to drug court program success**. | ||
189 | - Highlights **racial disparities in criminal justice-based rehabilitation programs**. | ||
190 | - Supports **policy discussions on improving access to drug treatment for marginalized groups**. | ||
191 | {{/expandable}} | ||
192 | |||
193 | {{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} | ||
194 | 1. Investigate **the role of mental health in drug court success rates**. | ||
195 | 2. Assess **long-term relapse prevention strategies post-treatment**. | ||
196 | 3. Explore **alternative diversion programs beyond traditional drug courts**. | ||
197 | {{/expandable}} | ||
198 | |||
199 | {{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} | ||
200 | [[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1081_JA-120014424.pdf]] | ||
201 | {{/expandable}} | ||
202 | {{/expandable}} | ||
203 | |||
204 | {{expandable summary=" | ||
205 | |||
206 | Study: Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?"}} | ||
207 | **Source:** *Intelligence (Elsevier)* | ||
208 | **Date of Publication:** *2014* | ||
209 | **Author(s):** *Michael A. Woodley, Jan te Nijenhuis, Raegan Murphy* | ||
210 | **Title:** *"Is there a Dysgenic Secular Trend Towards Slowing Simple Reaction Time?"* | ||
211 | **DOI:** [10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012](https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.05.012) | ||
212 | **Subject Matter:** *Cognitive Decline, Intelligence, Dysgenics* | ||
213 | |||
214 | {{expandable summary="📊 Key Statistics"}} | ||
215 | 1. **General Observations:** | ||
216 | - The study examines reaction time data from **13 age-matched studies** spanning **1884–2004**. | ||
217 | - Results suggest an estimated **decline of 13.35 IQ points** over this period. | ||
218 | |||
219 | 2. **Subgroup Analysis:** | ||
220 | - The study found **slower reaction times in modern populations** compared to Victorian-era individuals. | ||
221 | - Data from **Western countries (US, UK, Canada, Australia, Finland)** were analyzed. | ||
222 | |||
223 | 3. **Other Significant Data Points:** | ||
224 | - The estimated **dysgenic rate is 1.21 IQ points lost per decade**. | ||
225 | - Meta-regression analysis confirmed a **steady secular trend in slowing reaction time**. | ||
226 | {{/expandable}} | ||
227 | |||
228 | {{expandable summary="🔬 Findings"}} | ||
229 | 1. **Primary Observations:** | ||
230 | - Supports the hypothesis of **intelligence decline due to genetic and environmental factors**. | ||
231 | - Reaction time, a **biomarker for cognitive ability**, has slowed significantly over time. | ||
232 | |||
233 | 2. **Subgroup Trends:** | ||
234 | - A stronger **correlation between slower reaction time and lower general intelligence (g)**. | ||
235 | - Flynn effect (IQ gains) does not contradict this finding, as reaction time is a **biological, not environmental, measure**. | ||
236 | |||
237 | 3. **Specific Case Analysis:** | ||
238 | - Cross-national comparisons indicate a **global trend in slower reaction times**. | ||
239 | - Factors like **modern neurotoxin exposure** and **reduced selective pressure for intelligence** may contribute. | ||
240 | {{/expandable}} | ||
241 | |||
242 | {{expandable summary="📝 Critique & Observations"}} | ||
243 | 1. **Strengths of the Study:** | ||
244 | - **Comprehensive meta-analysis** covering over a century of reaction time data. | ||
245 | - **Robust statistical corrections** for measurement variance between historical and modern studies. | ||
246 | |||
247 | 2. **Limitations of the Study:** | ||
248 | - Some historical data sources **lack methodological consistency**. | ||
249 | - **Reaction time measurements vary by study**, requiring adjustments for equipment differences. | ||
250 | |||
251 | 3. **Suggestions for Improvement:** | ||
252 | - Future studies should **replicate results with more modern datasets**. | ||
253 | - Investigate **alternative cognitive biomarkers** for intelligence over time. | ||
254 | {{/expandable}} | ||
255 | |||
256 | {{expandable summary="📌 Relevance to Subproject"}} | ||
257 | - Provides evidence for **long-term intelligence trends**, contributing to research on **cognitive evolution**. | ||
258 | - Aligns with broader discussions on **dysgenics, neurophysiology, and cognitive load**. | ||
259 | - Supports the argument that **modern societies may be experiencing intelligence decline**. | ||
260 | {{/expandable}} | ||
261 | |||
262 | {{expandable summary="🔍 Suggestions for Further Exploration"}} | ||
263 | 1. Investigate **genetic markers associated with reaction time** and intelligence decline. | ||
264 | 2. Examine **regional variations in reaction time trends**. | ||
265 | 3. Explore **cognitive resilience factors that counteract the decline**. | ||
266 | {{/expandable}} | ||
267 | |||
268 | {{expandable summary="📄 Download Full Study"}} | ||
269 | [[Download Full Study>>attach:10.1016_j.intell.2014.05.012.pdf]] | ||
270 | {{/expandable}} | ||
271 | {{/expandable}} |